This repository contains the code for model fitting in our paper "Finding Structure in Multi-Armed Bandits".
Our behavioral analyises are hosted seperately here:
- Experiment 1: https://ericschulz.github.io/explin/exp1.html
- Experiment 2: https://ericschulz.github.io/explin/exp2.html
- Experiment 3: https://ericschulz.github.io/explin/exp3.html
- Experiment 4: https://ericschulz.github.io/explin/exp4.html
- Experiment 5: https://ericschulz.github.io/explin/exp5.html
The experiments themselves can be played by clicking the following links:
- Experiment 1: https://ericschulz.github.io/explin/index1.html
- Experiment 2: https://ericschulz.github.io/explin/index2.html
- Experiment 3: https://ericschulz.github.io/explin/index3.html
- Experiment 4: https://ericschulz.github.io/explin/index4.html
- Experiment 5: https://ericschulz.github.io/explin/index5.html
For the model fits, the main file of interest is
Model Fits and Parameter plots.ipynb
, which is a jupyter notebook with the results plotted.
Processing steps (run in order):
fit_clustering.py
: estimates the best alpha value for the clustering model for for each experiment using grid-searchget_bayesian_gp_means_std.py
: estimates a mixture of Gaussian-processes by inferring which of three kernels (RBF, Linear, Noise) to use at each moment in time using precomputed means and standard deviations of the modelsget_clustering_means_std.py
: estimates means and standard deviations of the clustering model.choice_models.py
: fits each of the 9 choice model using hierarchical Bayesian parameter estimation to each of the 5 experiments, saving the model fit statistics (LOO) and parameter estimates.run_ppc.py
: re-fits the GP-RBF/Clustering model and generates a sample from the posterior predictive distribution. Currently runs 2 out of 5 experiments (the linear and the shifted experiments)
N.B. Many of these files will take days to run on a modern laptop.
The model fitting uses means and standard deviations output from a Gaussian Process model,
which are stored in the folders Data/EXP_NAME
, as are the raw data files and the intermediate means
and standard deviations created by the code here. We used a separate process to generate the means
and standard deviations for the GPs, which required several days on a computing cluster. Breifly, the
a Gaussian process was fit to each trial for each subject given all of the observations the subject
had seen in that round of trials. Then, the predicted mean and stdev of the process were used as
predictors of the next trial.
Hierarchical Bayesian linear regression is used to fit the decision function to mean and standard
deviation of each arm of the bandit tasks. These means and standard deviations are separately estimated
(discussed below). All of the Bayesian models are defined in the file fit_clustering.py
using the PyMC3
library.
Each Bayesian model is more or less the same and the script includes preprocessing to unify the format of each of the tasks. The logic of the model is similar to multivariate regression, and we refer to the decision variables as "regression coefficents" and the values as "predictors" accordingly.
For each subject, a predictor vector is prepared for each model's mean and standard deviation for each trial. In addition a one-hot vector for sticky choice is also prepared. For each subject, a single (scalar valued) regression coefficient is multiplied to these vectors before they are added and passed through a softmax function. These parameters are fit 'hierachically', meaning we assume a group distribution over each parameter.
Example code for fitting the Kalman filter is provided below:
with pm.Model() as hier_kal:
mu_1 = pm.Normal('mu_beta_kal_mean', mu=0., sd=100.)
mu_2 = pm.Normal('mu_beta_kal_stdv', mu=0., sd=100.)
mu_3 = pm.Normal('mu_beta_stick', mu=0., sd=100.)
sigma_1 = pm.HalfCauchy('sigma_kal_means', beta=100)
sigma_2 = pm.HalfCauchy('sigma_kal_stdev', beta=100)
sigma_3 = pm.HalfCauchy('sigma_stick', beta=100)
b_1 = pm.Normal('beta_kal_mu', mu=mu_1, sd=sigma_1, shape=n_subj)
b_2 = pm.Normal('beta_kal_std', mu=mu_2, sd=sigma_2, shape=n_subj)
b_3 = pm.Normal('beta_sc', mu=mu_3, sd=sigma_3, shape=n_subj)
rho = \
tt.tile(tt.reshape(b_1[subj_idx], (n, 1)), d) * x_mu_kal + \
tt.tile(tt.reshape(b_2[subj_idx], (n, 1)), d) * x_sd_kal + \
tt.tile(tt.reshape(b_3[subj_idx], (n, 1)), d) * x_sc
p_hat = softmax(rho)
# Data likelihood
yl = pm.Categorical('yl', p=p_hat, observed=y)
# inference!
trace_kal = pm.sample(**sample_kwargs)
In this model, each of the three regression coefficients (b_1
, b_2
& b_3
for the means, standard
deviations and sticky choice, respectively) is assumed to be normally distributed. The means
(mu_1
, mu_2
, mu_3
) and standard deviations (sigma_1
, sigma_2
, sigma_3
)
of these distributions are group level parameters and can be interpreted as group effects. We've
assumed vague priors for these group level distributions, with the group means assumed to be
normally distributed and the standard deviations assumed to be distribued via a Half-cauchy
distribution (c.f. Gelman 2006 for more details)
PyMC3 currently uses the Theano tensor library to underly its calculatios. Because we have vectors as predictors, we have to use vector math to get the right representation for the softmax. The following codes casts the regresion coefficients into an n_arm by n_trial matrix so and uses element-wise multiplication to create the input to the softmax.
rho = \
tt.tile(tt.reshape(b_1[subj_idx], (n, 1)), d) * x_mu_kal + \
tt.tile(tt.reshape(b_2[subj_idx], (n, 1)), d) * x_sd_kal + \
tt.tile(tt.reshape(b_3[subj_idx], (n, 1)), d) * x_sc
p_hat = softmax(rho)
Below is the code for inference:
# Data likelihood
yl = pm.Categorical('yl', p=p_hat, observed=y)
# inference!
trace_kal = pm.sample(**sample_kwargs)
Most of this is automatically determined in PyMC3 and we encourage you to look at the documentation here for more details!